Keynote Speakers Archive

We’ve had the true honour of learning from some of the greats in the cognition and memory research field over the many years of NOWCAM. Scroll through our past keynote talks for inspiration!

2024: Peggy St. Jacques

University of Alberta

Perspectives and presence in event memory

Memories for events, such as autobiographical memories from our personal past, are immersive experiences. Not only do we form events from our personal past in an immersive surrounding centered on where we are in the scene, when we remember we also mentally place ourselves back in that event. This immersive nature of our memories gives rise to the subjective feeling of mental time travel and sense of presence within the past, which brings our memories “back to life.”

In this talk, I will discuss two separate lines of research examining the immersive nature of event memories. In the first part of the talk, I will discuss the role that our first-person viewpoint plays in shaping our memories. In the second part of the talk, I will discuss ongoing research in our lab investigating how the subjective sense of presence contributes to how we form and remember real-world events created using mixed reality.

Together this research will touch upon the cognitive neuroscience of how our self-perspective and sense of presence support immersive memories.

2022: Gary L. Wells

Iowa State University

Mistaken eyewitness identification and convictions of the innocent: A four-decade reflection on robust phenomena, safeguards, and frameworks

Reflecting on 40+ years of research on eyewitness identification, I will describe what I believe to be the three most reliable, robust, and expository phenomena in eyewitness identification from lineups. These three are the removal-without replacement effect, differential filler siphoning, and the post identification feedback effect.

These three phenomena, along with the failure to use double-blind eyewitness identification procedures with lineups, can explain almost every proven case of mistaken identification that resulted in convictions of innocent people.

In addition, I will describe a third category of variables that is importantly distinct from my original two-category distinction between system and estimator variables, namely reflector variables.

2020: Jason C.K. Chan

Iowa State University

Changing the past and future with retrieval practice

Retrieval can boost retention of studied material (i.e., the testing effect). An emerging literature shows that retrieving previously studied material can also promote future learning of new material (i.e., the forward testing effect).

I will introduce research on this phenomenon in the context of education and eyewitness memory. I will begin by discussing works that examine why retrieval promote future learning. I will then apply the understanding gained from this work to the context of eyewitness memory. Here, research has shown that performing a recall test (i.e., an eyewitness interview) can paradoxically increase a person’s susceptibility to later presented misinformation. I will examine the conditions under which this retrieval-enhanced suggestibility effect occurs and what can be done to prevent it.

More broadly, this research provides a window into understanding how retrieval affects retention (the past) and subsequent learning (the future).

2018: James W. Tanaka

University of Victoria

From milliseconds to decades: The creation and retrieval of holistic face memories

All human faces are created equal. We all share the same face parts (two eyes, a nose, and a mouth) arranged in a similar spatial configuration. Despite the perceptual similarity of faces, we are able to recognize the face of a friend, significant other, or colleague in a blink of an eye.

How is this impressive feat of visual recognition accomplished? Faces, unlike most objects, are encoded and retrieved as holistic memories. In my talk, I will discuss what a holistic face memory is. I will examine recent work in our lab suggesting that holistic face representations are formed in milliseconds and produce memories that could last a lifetime.

This research has implications for understanding how experience shapes the recognition of own- and other-race faces and how breakdowns in face recognition occur in autism and prosopagnosia.

2016: Heather Price

University of Regina

The justice of memory evidence

Memory evidence permeates the justice system. Despite decades of research designed to facilitate the reliable provision of such evidence, substantive gaps remain, many of which are a result of the field’s comfort with existing paradigms.

Dr. Price will discuss developments in the study of memory as evidence in the justice system and the growing movement towards questioning how we know what we know about the application of memory research to the law.

2014: D. Stephen Lindsay

University of Victoria

The source monitoring framework

How does the mind/brain differentiate fact from fancy, perception from inference? How do we distinguish creating a novel tune from remembering someone else’s melody? How do we discriminate memories of what we witnessed at the scene of a crime from memories of a co-witness’s description of what happened?

The central premise of the source-monitoring framework (SMF) is that the origins of our sensations, thoughts, images, and feelings are not abstractly and unambiguously specified and labelled a priori but rather are inferred by the mind/brain (usually very rapidly and without conscious reflection) on the basis of their content in the course of our experiencing them. Most of the time the inferences are correct, but often the accessed information is insufficient to support a source attribution and occasionally a mental event from one source is misattributed to another.

In the realm of gustatory experience, for example, inputs from the nose are routinely misattributed to the tongue; lacking olfaction it is reportedly difficult to distinguish an apple from a potato, but when one savours an orange pippin the lovely flavour sensations seem to come from the mouth.

As another perceptual example, what we see, hear, feel, or smell can be influenced by our expectations; if you are waiting for Don to telephone and your iPhone rings you may mistake Patrick’s voice for Don’s, especially if the connection quality is poor or there is lots of background noise or you are momentarily distracted as you take the call.

2012: Pierre Jolicoeur

University of Montreal

Cognitive neuroscience of attention and working memory: Some selective studies

Individuals who have sustained a mild brain injury (e.g., mild traumatic brain injury or mild cerebrovascular stroke) are at risk to show persistent cognitive symptoms (attention and memory) after the acute postinjury phase.

Although studies have shown that those patients perform normally on neuropsychological tests, cognitive symptoms remain present, and there is a need for more precise diagnostic tools.

The aim of this study was to develop precise and sensitive markers for the diagnosis of post brain injury deficits in visual and attentional functions which could be easily translated in a clinical setting.

Using electrophysiology, we have developed a task that allows the tracking of the processes involved in the deployment of visual spatial attention from early stages of visual treatment (N1, P1, N2, and P2) to higher levels of cognitive processing (no-go N2, P3a, P3b, N2pc, SPCN). This study presents a description of this protocol and its validation in 19 normal participants.

Results indicated the statistically significant presence of all ERPs aimed to be elicited by this novel task. This task could allow clinicians to track the recovery of the mechanisms involved in the deployment of visual-attentional processing, contributing to better diagnosis and treatment management for persons who suffer a brain injury.

2010: Geoffrey Loftus

University of Washington

How a cognitive psychologist can help a jury: Three examples from the real world of murder and mayhem

Witnesses to crimes and other forensically relevant events sometimes describe memories in which they express confidence that, on the basis of research in perception and memory, may be inappropriately high, given the circumstances of the case.

The most effective way of conveying this observation and the reasons for it to a jury is via an expert in perception and memory. In any given case, a mosaic of factors having to do with perception and memory are relevant.

I briefly sketch the most common of these factors and categorize them as to the degree to which they may be unambiguously and/or quantitatively applied to a specific case.

I discuss one such factor, witness–object distance, in some detail, showing how an expert might describe distance effects on perception and providing examples from actual cases.

2008: Tony Greenwald

University of Washington

Social cognition in the context of civil rights litigation

In the past few years, plaintiffs’ and defendants’ attorneys in class-action discrimination lawsuits have been applying pressure on social scientists to advance diametrically opposed positions concerning the effects of automatic associations on social behavior.

This talk describes challenges of balancing the roles of cautious scientist and expert witness, as well as some of the empirical findings that will likely play important roles in court cases.

2006: Eric Eich

University of British Columbia

Cognitive and clinical perspectives: Mood dependent memory

Recent years have witnessed a revival of research interest in the interplay between cognitive and emotional processes. Some of this interest has centered on mood dependent memory (MDM) — the observation that events experienced in a certain state of affect or mood are most retrievable in that mood.

In aid of better understanding MDM, researchers have pursued two distinct but complementary approaches. One approach features laboratory studies involving experimentally induced moods, and focuses on cognitive factors that play pivotal roles in the occurrence of mood dependence.

The second approach concentrates on clinical studies involving naturally occurring moods. The question of interest here is whether it is possible to demonstrate MDM in people who experience marked shifts in mood state as a consequence of a psychopathological condition, such as manic/depressive illness or multiple personality disorder.

In today’s talk, I will review recent research on both of these fronts, and discuss some of the advantages of studying MDM from both a cognitive and a clinical perspective.

2004: Jim Enns (Opening)

University of British Columbia

Constructing the present from the past with reentrant neural processes

When a mask follows a briefly presented target there are several consequences. The one that has historically received the most attention is a reduction in the visibility of the target. This is the conventional definition of masking.

Yet, another equally important consequence is that errors in target identification are biased toward the identity of the mask rather than being randomly distributed among the target alternatives. This is evidence of object substitution.

Finally, when the target is a signal to make a speeded action, this action can be influenced by a prime stimulus that is not even visible to the participant. This is known as masked response priming.

In this talk we review evidence concerning all three of these consequences of viewing rapid visual sequences. We argue that these consequences are difficult to understand, either individually or together, as the consequence of strictly feed-forward processing in the visual brain.

In contrast, when these results are considered from the perspective of reentrant visual circuitry, they are easier to understand and to relate to one another. Moreover, predictions derived from a reentrant view of the brain lead to unexpected and novel results that are confirmed when tested against psychophysical data.

2003: D. Stephen Lindsay

University of Victoria

Confidence and accuracy in eyewitnesses, investigators, and experts

We explored the effect of the degree of conceptual similarity between a witnessed event and an extra-event narrative on eyewitness suggestibility.

Experiments 1A and 1B replicated Allen and Lindsays (1998) finding that subjects sometimes intrude details from a narrative description of one event into their reports of a different visual event. Those experiments also showed that intrusion rates were even higher when the narrative described the visual event itself.

Experiment 2 replicated those findings, but found no more intrusions from a thematically similar versus dissimilar narrative.

In Experiment 3 we disguised the relationship between the narrative and visual event, and obtained more intrusions from a thematically similar than dissimilar narrative.

In Experiment 4 we obtained a thematic similarity effect when the relationship between narrative and visual event was disguised, but none when it was not. Results are discussed from the perspective of the source-monitoring framework.

2001: Bruce Whittlesea (Opening)

Simon Fraser University

Memo from mnemosyne: "If I look like twins, you're seeing double"

Many investigators have observed that the feeling of familiarity is associated with fluency of processing. The authors demonstrated a case in which the feeling of familiarity did not result from fluency per se; they argued that it resulted instead from perceiving a discrepancy between the actual and expected fluency of processing (B. W. A. Whittlesea & L. D. Williams, 1998).

In this talk, the authors extend that argument. They observed that stimuli that are experienced as strongly familiar when presented in isolation are instead experienced as being novel when presented in a rhyme or semantic context.

They interpreted that result to mean that in those other contexts, the subjects brought a different standard to bear in evaluating the fluency of their processing. This different standard caused the subjects to perceive their performance not as discrepant, but as coherent in one case and incongruous in the other.

The authors suggest that the perception of discrepancy is a major factor in producing the feeling of familiarity. They further suggest that the occurrence of that perception depends on the task in which the person is engaged when encountering the stimulus, because that task affects the standard that the person will apply in evaluating their processing.

2000: Jonathan Schooler

University of Pittsburgh

Meta-consciousness

A distinction is drawn between non-conscious (unexperienced), conscious (experienced), and meta-conscious (re-represented) mental processes.

There is evidence for two types of dissociations between consciousness and meta-consciousness, the latter being defined as the intermittent explicit re-representation of the contents of consciousness.

Temporal dissociations occur when an individual, who previously lacked meta-consciousness about the contents of consciousness, directs meta-consciousness towards those contents; for example, catching one’s mind wandering during reading.

Once meta-consciousness is triggered, translation dissociations can occur if the re-representation process misrepresents the original experience, such as when one verbally reflects on non-verbal experiences or takes stock of subtle or ambiguous experiences.

2023: Mike Caulfield

University of Washington

Beyond true and false: Perspectives from a decade teaching civic digital literacy

Much of digital information literacy was designed with the information seeker in mind. But on the social internet we do not only seek out information – information seeks out us as well, and often arrives missing crucial context.

In this keynote, information literacy expert Mike Caulfield will detail some of the ways in which pre-internet methods have not been suited to this reality, and detail insights that have emerged in the development and implementation of his SIFT methodology for “citizen fact-checking”.

Discussion will include the problem of “just doing the math”, the ways in which emotional reaction can be necessary to fact-checking, and how some of the older study of rumor may provide useful insights into how individuals process online information.

2021: Elizabeth Kensinger

Boston College

It's not all bad: How we remember positive events and find the good in the bad

The power of episodic memories is that they bring a past moment into the present, providing opportunities for us to recall details of the experiences, reframe or update the memory, and use the retrieved information to guide our decisions. In these regards, negative and positive memories can be especially powerful: Life’s highs and lows are disproportionately represented in memory, and when they are retrieved, they often impact our current mood and thoughts and influence various forms of behavior.

Research rooted in neuroscience and cognitive psychology has historically focused on memory for negative emotional content. Yet the study of autobiographical memories has highlighted the importance of positive emotional memories, and more recently, cognitive neuroscience methods have begun to clarify why positive memories may show powerful relations to mental wellbeing.

Here, we review the models that have been proposed to explain why emotional memories are long-lasting (durable) and likely to be retrieved (accessible), describing how in overlapping—but distinctly separable—ways, positive and negative memories can be easier to retrieve, and more likely to influence behavior. We end by identifying potential implications of this literature for broader topics related to mental wellbeing, education, and workplace environments.

2019: Ayanna Thomas

Tufts University

Memory reconstruction does not necessitate memory distortion: Learning to evaluate our subjective states

Rather than reproductive, research has consistently demonstrated that episodic memory is reconstructive in nature. Our experience of remembering episodic events seems to contribute to a cohesive and integrated narrative of our past; however, the cohesion is illusory.

Recollection involves the recombination of different pieces of our past experiences. These experiences, like puzzle pieces from different boxes, may be recombined correctly or incorrectly. Some pieces may be lost, eroded, or become in some other way, unavailable. Yet, when we recollect the past, we are evaluating this reconstructive process. We may monitor the ease with which information comes to mind. We may monitor the salience of specific attributes of a memory. We may consider how our internal states may have affected the quality of our reconstructed memories. And, ultimately, we decide whether these retrieved memories are accurate or inaccurate.

This evaluative process is metacognition, and failures in metacognitive processes contribute to memory reconstruction errors. We may misattribute feelings of familiarity, we may incorrectly evaluate the qualities of memories, or we may fail to exercise control over memory output. Frequently, however, the fallible nature of memory is predictable, and may be minimized, or at least, recognized by the individual. Metacognitive processes are central to understanding memory retrieval successes and failures.

What researchers have considered as evidence for the unreliable nature of memory (e.g., memory distortion, misinformation susceptibility, and even false memory creation), may be due to a failing of metacognitive processes. Therefore, metacognitive monitoring and control processes may serve as the foundation for avoiding the pitfalls of the reconstructive process. Memory is reconstructive. However, knowing this tenet gives us some control over retrieval. Reconstruction does not always need to result in distortion.

2017: Daniel M. Bernstein

Kwantlen Polytechnic University

I knew it and so did you! Social cognition across the lifespan

Social cognition permits us to communicate and empathize through our assessment of what others know and feel. Yet, our own knowledge and feelings often limit our ability to take another’s perspective, or know how another feels.

Our own knowledge can also limit our ability to recognize our own prior ignorance. These errors occur frequently in children, but also in adults. A challenge for social scientists is to develop tools and methods to study social cognition in children and adults.

I will present work exploring social cognition from preschool to old age. Fusing developmental, cognitive, and learning sciences, this research can benefit researchers, teachers, students, policy makers and parents.

2015: Norman Brown

University of Alberta

Transition theory: Autobiographical memory from the bottom-up

Our lives are boring, or at least repetitive. Oddly enough, this basic fact has been ignored by researchers interested in autobiographical memory. In this article, I summarize a theory, Transition Theory (T²), developed in response to this situation; I also review a project, the Living-in-History project, which motivated it.

In brief, the Living-in-History project demonstrates that public events occasionally play an important role in how people think about their lives. However, this happens only where (and when) these events produce a fundamental and enduring change in the fabric of daily life. T² provides a psychologically tractable formulation for this metaphor.

On this view, the “threads” that constitute the fabric of daily life correspond to mental representations that capture our knowledge of repeatedly encountered people, places, and things and recurring activities. During stable times, these representations are “woven” together by basic associative processes and come to form lifetime periods (i.e., “my college years,” “when I was living in New York,” etc.). These period representations are separated (delineated) by major life transitions.

Transitions are events like relocation or the birth of a child which “change everything.” Follow along with the fabric metaphor, transitional events replace one set of threads (i.e., the frequently encountered, material features of everyday life) with another, and do so in a rapid, exhaustive, and synchronized manner.

Thus, T² contends that autobiographical memory is structured by important life transitions and that major transitions play an important role in organizing memories, regardless of whether they are individual (e.g., immigration) or collective (e.g., war) in nature.

2013: Adele Diamond

University of British Columbia

Leveraging what we've learned from brain research to help every child succeed

Adele Diamond is the Canada Research Chair Professor of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience at the University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada. She received her B.A. from Swarthmore College Phi Beta Kappa (in Sociology-Anthropology & Psychology), her Ph.D. from Harvard (in Developmental Psychology), and was a postdoctoral fellow at Yale Medical School in Neuroanatomy.

A leader in two fields, psychology and neuroscience, Dr. Diamond is at the forefront of research on the most complex human abilities (collectively called ‘executive functions,’ which include attention, selfcontrol, & reasoning). Her work has changed the medical treatment for two different medical disorders.

Recently Dr. Diamond has turned her attention to the possible roles of play, dance, music, and storytelling in improving executive functions, academic outcomes and mental health.

2011: Daniel Richardson

University College London

Eye and thou: Eye movements and social cognition

Movements of the eye are determined by an interaction of low level properties of the stimulus and high level cognitive factors. Typically in eye movement research, the cognitive factors are that are investigated are expectations or schemas for particular types of scene.

I will present three projects demonstrating that social factors also have a substantial contribution to eye movements. In the first, participants watched a video of people giving their views on a sensitive political issue.

One speaker made a potentially offensive remark. If participants believed these remarks could be heard by others, they fixated individuals who were likely to be offended. In a second study, two participants in adjacent cubicles had a discussion over an intercom while they were eye tracked. We found that their gaze coordination was modulated by what each believed the other could see on the computer screen.

In the final set of experiments, we simply showed groups of four stimuli to pairs of participants. We found that that individuals looked at photographs differently if they believed that the other person was looking at the same images as them rather than a set of random symbols.

Together these experiments demonstrate that social forces have a strong effect on perceptual mechanisms. Gaze patterns are determined by what we think others will feel, what we think our conversation partners can see, and simply whether or not we think we are looking alone or with other people.

2009: Marryanne Garry

University of Waikato

Memory: Change we can believe in

Maryanne Garry received her Ph.D. in 1993 from the University of Connecticut, and did postdoctoral work at the University of Washington.

In 1996, she moved to Victoria University of Wellington, where she worked for 20 years before taking up a joint appointment in 2016 at the University of Waikato, as a Professor of Psychology and a Professor in the New Zealand Institute for Security and Crime Science (one of only two in the world) at The University of Waikato.

She studies a puzzle of memory: how is that otherwise intelligent, rational people can remember or believe things about themselves, or about others, that just aren’t true?

Her work has been funded by granting agencies in the U.S., Japan, and the New Zealand Government through the Marsden Fund, administered by the Royal Society of New Zealand on behalf of the Marsden Fund Council.

2007: Vincent Di Lollo

Simon Fraser University

Memory and prediction: That's what the brain is in business for

In agreement with neuroanatomical evidence, but contrary to conventional feed-forward notions, my colleagues and I hold to a scheme in which perceptions emerge from iterative exchanges between brain regions linked by reentrant pathways.

In this scheme, the brain is seen as a repository of memories in the form of neural networks (cell assemblies and phase sequences) established through Hebbian learning. Those networks are used in cortical reentrant loops to set up moment-to-moment action plans for perceiving objects and for predicting behaviour sequences.

Long-standing problems, including the development of perceptual categories and the “binding” problem, are resolved naturally within this conceptual framework.

I will illustrate this viewpoint with evidence from behavioural manifestations such as visual masking, and electrophysiological evidence from MEG and event-related potentials that provide converging evidence for a reentrant theory of perception and cognition.

2005: Alan Kingstone

University of British Columbia

Cognitive ethology: Giving real life to attention research

Studies of attention, often conducted in artificial laboratory experiments, may have limited validity when performance in the natural world is considered.

For instance, for over two decades, investigations of ‘reflexive’ and ‘volitional’ attention have tended to be grounded in methodologies that do not capture the demands of attention in everyday life.

Recent studies suggest these laboratory investigations have lost touch with real-life contexts and accordingly may generate fundamental misunderstandings regarding the principles of human attention and behavior.

This talk identifies the basic assumptions of laboratory research that has led to this state of affairs, and suggests a new set of assumptions which lead to a new research approach called ‘cognitive ethology’. The implication is that if one is to understand human attention in everyday life, then research needs to be grounded in the natural world and not in experimental paradigms.

2004: Lee Osterhout (Closing)

University of Washington

The view from the scalp: Correcting our vision of language by recording event-related brain potentials

Adult second-language (L2) learning is often claimed to be slow and laborious compared to native language (L1) acquisition, but little is known about the rate of L2 word learning.

Here we discuss that adult second-language learners’ brain activity, as measured by event-related potentials (ERPs), discriminated between L2 words and L2 ‘pseudowords’ (word-like letter strings) after just 14 h of classroom instruction.

This occurred even while the learners performed at chance levels when making overt L2 word-nonword judgments, indicating that the early acquisition of some aspects of a new language may be overlooked by current behavioral assessments.

2002: J. Don Read

Simon Fraser University

From the lab to the great beyond: Generalizing basic memory research to other-world settings

Dr. J. Don Read is Professor of Psychology and Director of the Law and Forensic Psychology Program at Simon Fraser University. Dr. Read received a B.A. degree from the University of British Columbia followed by M.Sc. and Ph.D. degrees from Kansas State University.

His research investigates eyewitness memory, face recognition, reconstructive memory, recollections of childhood abuse, and long-term autobiographical memory and has been funded by the National Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, NATO, and the Alberta Law Foundation.

Dr. Read has published some 70 papers and chapters and co-edited four other books including Recollections of Trauma (1997), Eyewitness Memory (1997), and Adult Eyewitness Memory (1994).

Dr. Read was recently the Chair of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (1998–2002) and the North American Editor of Applied Cognitive Psychology. He is currently Editorial Board member of Applied Cognitive Psychology, Law and Human Behavior, Legal and Criminological Psychology, and The Oxford Press Psychology and Law Book Series.

2001: Daniel Reisberg (Closing)

Reed College

Locating (and escaping) the limits on image-based discovery

Daniel Reisberg is an American academic who is the Patricia and Clifford Lunneborg Professor of Psychology at Reed College in Portland, Oregon. His specialty is cognitive psychology, and he focuses on memory, judgment, and imagery, particularly in relation to emotion.

Reisberg earned his B.A. in psychology and philosophy from Swarthmore College in 1975. He then attended the University of Pennsylvania, obtaining an M.A. in 1976 and a Ph.D. in 1980. Reisberg has been teaching at Reed College since 1986.

Reisberg is the author of The Science of Perception and Memory: A Pragmatic Guide for the Justice System (Oxford University Press). Reisberg also consults widely with the justice community, and occasionally appears in court as an expert witness.

1999: Elizabeth F. Loftus

University of Washington

Repressed memories: When are they real? How are they false?
Assuming that some forgotten experiences can later be remembered, there is still a separate and rather crucial question to consider.
Namely, can people come to believe that they had experiences when they did not? Can they falsely “remember” something that might be highly upsetting if it had happened, such as sexual abuse?
Numerous researchers have explored the issue of whether false memories can be planted in the minds of ordinary people.